text
stringlengths 0
2.12k
|
---|
Participants |
I solicited survey responses from 66 undergraduate Dartmouth students. I set up a table at the |
bottom of the Berry Library staircase alongside the Novak Café for a total of four hours between |
1:30 and 3:30 PM on Tuesday, November 9 and Wednesday, November 10. I approached |
students to take a survey and ensured that each participant was a student that I had never met |
before to mitigate selection bias. Two survey responses had to be discarded because of |
incomplete information. Accordingly, the compiled results consist of data from 64 students. |
Materials |
I created a survey to get data on the variables in question and control for any confounding |
variables. The survey included five questions. The first question inquired about a participant’s |
social network structure: |
How would you classify your friends? |
a. Most of my friends are friends with each other |
b. Most of my friends are in a few different groups |
c. Most of my friends are independent from one another |
Each letter response represents one of the social network structures in McCabe’s model. Option |
A describes a tight-knitter, option B points to a compartmentalizer, and option C is indicative of |
a sampler. Although McCabe’s model only has empirical support in an offline study, the |
literature suggests that offline and online social network structures are analogous to one another |
(Dunbar et al., 2015). If the literature is correct and McCabe’s model can be applied to online |
social network structures, then I expect to see the number of online relationships that students |
have vary across options A, B, and C. |
The second question solicited the number of Instagram followers a participant has. According to |
a study of social media use among teens, Instagram is the most popular social media with 72% of |
teen social media users being active on the Instagram platform (Anderson et al., 2018). For this |
reason, Instagram was my platform of choice for gauging digital social networks. The number of |
Instagram followers that a participant has is representative of the size of their online social |
network. Followers, as opposed to the number of users that the participant is following, are |
willingly a part of the participant’s social network and are therefore a good indicator of the scale |
of a participant’s online social network. |
The third question asked for a participant’s GPA. Responses from this question were used as a |
measure of academic performance. The fourth and fifth questions recorded a participant’s major |
area of study and class year of graduation, respectively. These last two questions were added to |
control for the influence of major and class year on experimental results. |
Procedure |
Experimental Design – I loaded the survey on a tablet during the data collection timeframes |
noted above and approached unfamiliar students one at a time. If a student consented to being |
part of the study, I handed them the tablet and asked them to fill out the survey. I noted that data was anonymous and gave them privacy while they recorded their responses. I elected to conduct |
the survey digitally and self-report style to minimize the time cost of data collection and |
maximize the number of willing participants. If a student did not have an Instagram account or a |
GPA, I still asked them to record their response and discarded their data later on. I budgeted four |
hours for data collection and stopped accepting responses afterwards. |
Statistical Analysis – I compiled the data into a spreadsheet and categorized it according to |
reported social network structure. No participants identified as samplers; they did not record that |
their friends are independent from one another. Since the data only comprised two social |
network structures, these variables were independent, they had equal variances, and the data |
came from two randomly sampled normal populations, I decided to use a two-sample t-test to |
determine the significance of varying Instagram followers and GPA across network structures |
The responses of 64 students were collected and analyzed. 41 participants identified as tight- |
knitters, 23 participants described themselves as compartmentalizers, and 0 participants |
classified themselves as samplers. Statistical analyses returned evidence that GPA and the |
number of Instagram followers that students have vary significantly across social network |
structures. |
H1 (Instagram Followers) – The 23 participants who described themselves as |
compartmentalizers (M = 1460.43, SD = 564.66) compared to the 41 participants who classified |
themselves as tight-knitters (M = 1249.88, SD = 364.43) demonstrated significantly larger online |
social networks, t(62) = 1.81, p < 0.05. On average, compartmentalizers had more Instagram |
followers than tight-knitters. Thus, we reject H1’s null hypothesis and conclude that we have |
evidence that McCabe’s social network structure model is applicable to online social networks. |
Students with different social network structures exhibit variance in academic |
performance and the size of their online social network. Specifically, compartmentalizers have |
significantly larger online social networks and higher GPAs than tight-knitters. Participants’ |
major area of study and graduation year were not found to have any confounding influence on |
the results. Evidence from these studies supports two conclusions: |
C1: McCabe’s social network model involves meaningful structural distinctions |
that are realized in online social networks |
C2: the structure of a student’s offline or online social network may play a role |
in shaping their academic performance |
These conclusions have far-reaching consequences. C1 is encouraging for |
extending applications of McCabe’s model from just offline social networks to both |
offline and online social networks. It also illustrates that college students tend to |
translate structural features of their in-person social networks to their digital social |
networks. C2, in conjunction with C1, indicates that who a student knows and how they |
know them, whether it be in-person or through social media, might be a determinant of |
the grades they receive. Since we have evidence of the structural parallels between |
offline and online social networks, we can reason that those structures are more or less |
similar and that the overarching structure could influence academic performance. |
In particular, it appears that a compartmentalized social network structure is |
more academically advantageous than a tight-knit social network structure. On average, |
compartmentalizers have more online relationships and higher GPAs than tight-knitters. |
I hypothesize that compartmentalizers have more friends with more diverse academic |
interests – and therefore more academic support – than tight-knitters because they draw |
friendships from different domains. Tight-knitters seek out friendships in a closed |
network; if some resource does not exist in that network, say the answer to a math |
problem or notes from biology class, then the tight-knitter has no way of getting the help |
Subsets and Splits